Clientelism is the exchange of goods and services involving an implicit or explicit quid-pro-quo. In science it has come to mean pseudoscience consisting of reports in scientific journals designed a priori to advance commercial sources of research funding. Some of the earliest examples of this form of clientelism involved funding by the tobacco industry pseudoscience that questioned the link between cigarette smoking and bronchogenic carcinoma of the lung. Clientelism was wide-spread between investigators and the athletic footwear industry commencing 40 years ago. University based investigators were essentially hired by running shoe manufacturers to publish reports in scientific journals as if it was truly independent research with the implicit or explicit understanding that there were to portray running shoes as offering protection against injury through such means as "cushioning" and "frontal plane foot movement control" despite evidence that injuries remained frequent with their use. This clientelism diminished when it this clientelism was exposed thereby exposing major footwear manufacturers at risk of litigation for false advertising. They began then selling their products almost exclusively through their appearance (fashion; aesthetics) and athlete endorsement. Clientelism returned with the advent of the "minimalist" or "barefoot" shoe, via smaller athletic footwear manufacturers that seemed unaware of the risk of litigation for false advertising. These footwear have been falsely advertised as accurately simulating barefoot locomotion, thereby providing protection offered by the bare foot through natural selection. The first example was the work of of Daniel Lieberman which was supported by Vibram (maker of FiveFingers), a footwear manufacturer. This resulted in class action lawsuit which has recently been settled. It continues. The following is a letter I wrote to the Editor-in-Chief of the European Journal of Sports Science. It deals with a report authored by Zech,
A., Argubi-Wollesen, A., & Rahlf, A of the Fredrich-Schiller-Universitat Jena, Institut für Sportwissenschaft, which was published in 2015. It was supported by, Leguano GmbH, a footwear manufacturer, that makes un-cushioned minimalist shoes. To the Editor-in-Chief:
The report by Zech et al. (Zech, Argubi-Wollesen, & Rahlf, 2015), examined stability when a highly stable cohort was barefoot and used selected athletic footwear, as so many others have done commencing more than two decades ago (Robbins, Waked, & McClaren, 1992; Robbins, Waked, Gouw, & McClaren, 1994; Franklin, Grey, Heneghan, Bowen, & Li, 2015). But their results differ from most reports in that they found stability was better when wearing shoes - the opposite of what has been reported in all age cohorts except for the extremely old (Robbins, Waked, Gouw, & McClaren, 1992). This can be accounted for by the method they used to infer stability which is invalid. Also, Zech et al. seem to exist in a solitude whereby their report is not placed in context of other similar reports, nor into our current understanding of mechanisms humans use to maintain stable equilibrium.
High stability in humans is the state of weight-bearing with low risk of falling, hence the most valid means of measuring it is quantifying falls under conditions of daily life. This is impractical particularly in stable cohorts, as Zech et al. use, because they rarely fall. Alternatively, stability can be inferred by measures that have been shown to correlate well with falls risk, such as falls from a narrow beam when walking when considering a stable cohort, or falls with one footed standing with less stable cohorts (Robbins, Waked & McClaren, 1992). Stability testing using validated methods have shown that stability in humans progressively declines commencing at 40 years of age – the age when falls risk increases in healthy humans, and footwear impair stability in all but the oldest cohort (Robbins, Waked, Allard & Krouglicoff, 1997).
Zech et al. use a six degree of freedom (x, y, z, pitch, yaw, roll) force platform to infer stability. Force platform output indicates displacement of center of mass over time, which has been expressed in a multitude of similar measures, which in totality are called “sway measures” or simply “sway”. Sway is continuous when standing, but this behavior is not only postural adjustments to maintain stable equilibrium as Zech et al. presume (Duarte, & Sternad, 2008; Loram, Maganaris, & Lakie, 2009). For example, sway declines with each advancing trimester of gestation as fall risk increases (Dunning, Lemasters, & Bhattacharya, 2010; McCrory, Chambers, Daftery, & Redfern, 2010; Olivera, Viera, Macedo, Simpson, & Nadal, 2009; Evenson, & Wen, 2011). Furthermore, when the most stable age cohort stand unstressed (on two feet; eyes open; a firm support base), sway is high and increases further with prolonged standing - often exceeding levels seen in the unstable elderly who are at high risk of falling. Surely any method that is incapable of distinguishing stable from unstable cohorts must be considered invalid. Sway with low stress in stable cohorts is best explained as mainly a measure of plantar load redistribution behavior that protects the plantar skin from ischemic necrosis. As balance stress rises, humans first choose to lower falls risk through reduction of plantar protective behavior to a point where prolonged standing plantar discomfort encourages them to become sedentary. Sway becomes positively related to falls risk once plantar skin protective behavior is extinguished which is nearly impossible to determine with precision, or if specific protocols are validated against actual falls risk in the cohort tested, which Zech et al. did not do. The best available explanation of their results which is consistent with previous reports is that, considering that one-footed standing is not particularly stressful in the cohort they chose to test, the barefoot condition was associated with the greatest sway but also the greatest stability because it was a measure of plantar load redistribution behavior. This is the opposite of Zech et al.'s conclusions.
Zech et al. fail to report developments in the last 25 years regarding how humans use plantar tactile influences in maintaining stable equilibrium. We now know that humans parsimoniously use plantar tactile information mainly from SA II mechanoreceptors to maintain superior stable equilibrium when barefoot but not with footwear), but also to control impact (Robbins, Hanna, & Jones; Robbins & Waked, 1998), and to provide precise foot position awareness (Bart, Van Wezel, Ottenhoff, & Duysens, 1997; Robbins, & Waked, 1998; Robbins, Waked, & Rappel, 1998). Evidence from this comes from the known effect of footwear on plantar shear-stress which is an adequate stimulus for SA II mechanoreceptors, particularly when combined with normal loading (Iglis, Kennedy, Wells, & Chua , 2002; Kennedy, & Inglis, 2002). Direct recording from primary afferents of plantar surface SA II mechanoreceptors support the above analyses (Iglis, Kennedy, Wells, & Chua, 2002; Kennedy, & Inglis, 2002). Also, stability with athletic footwear is positively and negatively related to sole hardness and sole thickness, respectively (Robbins, Waked, & McClaren, 1992). It has been shown that stability is positively related to foot position awareness, which correlates well with frontal plane foot movement when bearing weight on one foot (Robbins, Waked, & McClaren, 1995; Robbins, Waked, Allard, & Krouglicoff, 1998). This led to the hypothesis that athletic shoes destabilize through creating an under-damped state of frontal plane oscillation (Robbins, Waked, & Krouglicoff, 1998). This hypothesis was tested in a report in a geriatrics journal with sole materials differing in resiliency. It was found that stability improves with extremely low resiliency sole materials which produce an over-damped condition (Robbins, Waked, & Krouglicoff, 1998). There is evidence that decline in stability when barefoot with age is likely caused by SA II receptor decline, worsened by footwear created forefoot osteoarthritis in the extremely old which is the only cohort in which barefoot stability is worse than when wearing shoes (Robbins, Waked, & McClaren,1995; Machado, Bombach, Duysens, &Carpes, 2016).
It seems unlikely that Zech et al. are totally unaware force platform limitations of inferring stability, and must be knowledgeable of the above relevant published reports, but chose to use them highly selectively. For example, they mention reports that support their claims (Federolf, Roos, & Nigg, 2012; Koepell, et al., 2004), but not a host of others that refute it (Franklin, Grey, Heneghan, Bowen, & Li, 2015). This suggests that both choice of method and literature review may have been a strategy.
I find it revealing that they conclude:
“….the findings do not support the hypothesis that barefoot standing is beneficial for static postural control. Moreover, the fact that un-cushioned minimalist shoes with direct skin contact at the foot and ankle joint resulted in the lowest postural sway during standing …....”
It may simply be a coincidence that their source of funding was Leguano GmbH, a footwear manufacturer, that makes un-cushioned minimalist shoes, and their conclusions seem to promote the use of these products as offering superior stability. Are Zech et al. involved in clientelism through, on a quid pro quo basis, pandering to their source of funding?
There is a pressing need to further our understanding of human stability particularly in relation to footwear, a relatively recent development in human history that is mainly based on an aesthetic tradition. They substantially affect stability and gait. But scientific advancement requires that authors remain objective and avoid clientelism with their funding sources. The resulting pseudo-scientific advertising presented as original research diminishes the scientific journals which publish reports in good faith, and the authors that honestly report their results in them.
References
1 Bart, M., Van Wezel, H., Ottenhoff, F., Duysens, J. (1997) Dynamic Control of Location-Specific Information in Tactile Cutaneous Reflexes from the Foot during Human Walking. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(10), 3804-3814. 2 Duarte, M., Sternad, D. (2008) Complexity of human postural control in young and older adults during prolonged standing. Experimental Brain Research, 191, 265-276. 3 Dunning, K. LeMasters, G., Bhattacharya, A. (2010) A Major Public Health Issue: The High Incidence of Falls during Pregnancy. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 14, 720-725. 4 Evenson, K., Wen, F. (2011) Prevalence and correlates of objectively measured physical activity and sedentary behavior among US pregnant women. Preventive Medicine, 53, 39-43. 5 Federolf, P., Roos, L., Nigg, B. (2012). The effect of footwear on postural control in bipedal quiet stance. Footwear Science, 4, 115-122. 6 Franklin,S., Grey, M.J., Heneghan N., Bowen, L,. Li, F. (2015) Barefoot vs common footwear: A systematic review of the kinematic, kinetic and muscle activity differences during walking. Gait & Posture, 42(3), 230-239. 7 Inglis, J.T., Kennedy, P.M., Wells, C., Chua, R. (2002) The role of cutaneous receptors in the foot. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, 508, 111-117. 8 Kennedy, P.M., Inglis, J.T. (2002) Distribution and behaviour of glabrous cutaneous receptors in the human foot sole. Journal of Physiology, 538, 995-1002. 9 Koepell, T.D., Wolf, M.E., Buchner, D.M., KuKull, W.A., LaCroix, A.Z., Tencer, A.F., Frankenfeld, C.L. Tautvydas, M., Larson, E.B. (2004) Footwear style and risk of falls in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 52(9), 1495-501. 10 Loram, I., Maganaris, C., Lakie, M. (2009) Paradoxical muscle movement during postural control. Medicine and Science in Sports & Exercise, 41, 198-204. 11 Machado, A.S., Bombach, G.D., Duysens, J., Carpes, F.P. (2016) Difference in foot sensitivity and plantar pressure between young adults and elderly. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 63, 67-71. 12 McCrory, J.L., Chambers, A.J., Daftary, A., Redfern, M.S. (2010) Dynamic postural stability during advancing pregnancy. Journal of Biomechanics, 117, 2434-2439. 13 Oliveira, L.F., Vieira, T.M.M., Macedo, A.R., Simpson, D.M., Nadal, J. (2009) Postural sway changes during pregnancy: A descriptive study using stabilometry. European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology, 147(1), 25-28. 14 Pollard, J.P., Le Quesne L.P., Tappin, J.W. (1983) Forces under the foot. Journal of Biomedical Engineering, 5(1), 37-40. 15 Robbins, S.E., Hanna, A., Jones, L.A. (1988) Sensory attenuation induced by modern athletic footwear. (1988) Journal of Testing and Evaluation, 16(4), 412-416. 16 Robbins, S.E, Waked, E. (1997) Foot position awareness: the effect of footwear on instability, excessive impact, and ankle spraining. Critical Reviews in Physical Rehabilitative Medicine, 9, 53-74. 17 Robbins, S.E., Waked, E. (1998) Factors Associated with Ankle Injuries. Sports Medicine, 25, 63-72. 18 Robbins, S.E., Waked, E., Allard, P., Krouglicoff, N. (1997) Foot position awareness in older men: the influence of shoe sole material. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 45, 61-66. 19 Robbins, S.E., Waked, E., Gouw, G.J., & McClaren, J. (1994). Athletic footwear affect balance in men. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 28(2), 117–122. 20 Robbins, S.E., Waked, E., Krouglicof, N. (1998) Improving balance. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 46, 1363-70. 21 Robbins, S.E., Waked, E., McClaren, J. (1992 ) Shoe Sole Thickness and Hardness Influence Balance in Older Men. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40(11), 1089-1094. 22 Robbins, S.E., Waked, E., McClaren, J. (1995) Propriocepion and stability: Foot position awareness as a function of age and footwear. Age and Ageing, 24, 67-72. 23 Robbins, S.E., Waked, E., Rappel, R. (1995) Ankle taping improves proprioception before and after exercise in young men. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 29, 242-247. 24 Waked, E., Robbins, S.E. (1997) The effect of footwear midsole hardness and thickness on proprioception and stability in older men. Journal of Testing and Evaluation, 25:143. 25 Zech, A., Argubi-Wollesen, A., & Rahlf, A. (2015). Minimalist, standard and no footwear on static and dynamic postural stability following jump landing. European Journal of Sports and Exercise Science, 15(4), 279-285. |